Skip to main content

Characterization of cognitive deficits in spontaneously hypertensive rats, accompanied by brain insulin receptor dysfunction

Abstract

Background

The spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) has been used to model changes in the central nervous system associated with cognitive-related disorders. Recent human and animal studies indicate a possible relationship between cognitive deficits, insulin resistance and hypertension. We aimed to investigate whether cognitively impaired SHRs develop central and/or peripheral insulin resistance and how their cognitive performance is influenced by the animal’s sex and age as well as strains used for comparison (Wistar and Wistar-Kyoto/WKY).

Methods

Three and seven-month-old SHR, Wistar, and WKY rats were studied for their cognitive performance using Morris Water Maze (MWM) and Passive Avoidance tests (PAT). Plasma glucose and insulin were obtained after oral glucose tolerance tests. Cerebral cortex, hippocampus, and striatum status of insulin-receptor (IR) β-subunit and glycogen synthase kinase-3β (GSK3β) and their phosphorylated forms were obtained via ELISA.

Results

SHRs performed poorly in MWM and PAT in comparison to both control strains but more pronouncedly compared to WKY. Females performed poorer than males and 7-month-old SHRs had poorer MWM performance than 3-month-old ones. Although plasma glucose levels remained unchanged, plasma insulin levels were significantly increased in the glucose tolerance test in 7-month-old SHRs. SHRs demonstrated reduced expression and increased activity of IRβ-subunit in cerebral cortex, hippocampus, and striatum with different regional changes in phospho/total GSK3β ratio, as compared to WKYs.

Conclusion

Results indicate that cognitive deficits in SHRs are accompanied by both central and peripheral insulin dysfunction, thus allowing for the speculation that SHRs might additionally be considered as a model of insulin resistance-induced type of dementia.

Background

The spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) was inbred from the Wistar-Kyoto rat strain (WKY) [1]. SHRs were initially developed as a model for hypertension but were eventually shown to develop pathologies at the metabolic, behavioral, and cognitive level. The metabolic pathology is manifested as systemic insulin resistance [2]; the behavioral symptoms resemble attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (hyperactivity, impulsivity, impaired ability to withhold responses, and poorly sustained attention) [35]; and the cognitive pathology is manifested as learning and memory deficits [68]. Several studies have explored the link between an insulin resistant brain state and cognitive impairment, particularly in dementia of the Alzheimer type as reviewed elsewhere [911]. Insulin and insulin receptors (IR) in the brain have been shown to regulate glucose metabolism [12], as expected, but IR also triggers complex signaling pathways in the brain [1317]. These pathways involve glycogen synthase-kinase-3 (GSK3) inhibition, via protein kinase B (Akt/PKB) [16, 17]. In addition, IR influences the accumulation/degradation of amyloid-β and tau protein, the major neuropathological hallmarks of the memory loss developed in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [1315]. We aimed to investigate whether cognitively impaired SHRs develop a central insulin resistance in parallel to the peripheral one, but data published so far on cognitive performance in the SHR model have indicated that measurements were done using different cognitive tests, comparing different control strains, using animals of different ages and not always done using both sexes [3, 1832]. Since these factors might interfere with cognitive performance in rats, we have additionally explored their possible influence on learning and memory functions in the SHR model.

In brief, following the comparison between different control strains and sexes which showed that female SHRs demonstrate more pronounced cognitive deficits than males, particularly in comparison to the WKY controls, our studies showed that cognitive deficits in female SHRs are accompanied by peripheral and central insulin resistance in 7- but not in 3-month old animals in comparison to WKY controls.

Methods

Animals

The experiments were performed on Wistar SHR and 2 normotensive control strains, Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) and Wistar rats, all purchased from Charles River Laboratories (Kisslegg, Germany). Animals were acclimated for 2 weeks before cognitive testing which was performed at the Department of Pharmacology, University of Zagreb School of Medicine (Zagreb, Croatia). Animals consumed standardized food pellets and water ad libitum.

Ethics

All procedures were performed under the guidance of the Principles of Laboratory Animal Care (NIH Publication No. 80-23, revised in 1996) and in accordance with the European Communities Council Directive of 24 November 1986 (86/609/EEC) and the Croatian Act on Animal Welfare (NN 135/2006). All experiments were approved by the University of Zagreb School of Medicine (Licence No.04-1343-2006).

Experimental design

Three different experiments were performed in vivo to explore the effect of (1) control rat strain, with 12-week male SHR, WKY and Wistar rats, (2) sex, with 12-week-old female and male SHR and WKY rats, and (3) age, with 12- and 28-week-old (i.e. 3- and 7-month-old) female SHR and WKY rats. Animal age in the third experiment corresponded to young adult and older, “middle-aged” humans [33] as well as represented the periods of rising blood pressure (3 months) and sustained/chronic hypertension (7 months) [34, 35]. There were 10 animals per group in all experiments.

Cognitive testing

Morris Water Maze (MWM) swimming test

The MWM tested learning ability and spatial memory as previously described and used in our experiments [36, 37] (details provided in the Additional file 1). The time needed to find the platform (seconds) and the number of errors (incorrect entries into quadrants with no platform) were recorded in training trials during 4 consecutive days while the time spent searching for the platform after entering quadrant IV and number of errors were recorded in the probe trial that followed.

Passive Avoidance test (PAT)

The step-through PAT was performed two days after finishing the MWM test and exploited a fear-motivated tendency of a rat to escape from an illuminated area into a dark area as previously described and used in our experiments [36, 37]. Latency time (seconds) before entering the dark area was recorded on the third testing day. A more detailed PAT method is provided in the Additional file 1.

Oral glucose tolerance test

The OGTT was performed after the rats were under deep chloral hydrate (300 mg/kg i.p.) anesthesia, in accordance with the ethical requirements for laboratory animal procedures of the Medical School University of Zagreb. Blood samples were sequentially collected from the tail vein (50 μL; between 8 and 12 a.m.) for plasma glucose measurements at baseline, 30, and 60 min after the challenge. The post-OGTT cut-off time of 60 min corresponded to the duration of anesthesia from a single chloral hydrate dose. Animals were sacrificed after the last blood withdrawal still in deep anesthesia, 2 days after last cognitive testing.

Preparation of brain regions

Macrodissection of three brain regions (frontal cortex, striatum, and hippocampus) was performed on a cold plate (4 °C) by a standard procedure and according to the rat brain atlas [38]. The brain samples were snap frozen and stored at -80 °C until further analysis.

Brain homogenates

Frozen brain tissue from each rat was weighed, and a 4-fold concentration of RIPA buffer (Sigma-Aldrich, Schelldorf, Germany) was added with protease and phosphatase inhibitors (Sigma-Aldrich, Schelldorf, Germany). Samples were homogenized via ultrasonic disruption (15 % amplitude, 10 s) at 4 °C. After homogenization, the probes were centrifuged at 44,000 × g at 4 °C for 10 min. Clear supernatant (which contained proteins) was collected into a fresh tube for further analysis.

Protein concentration

For each sample, protein concentrations were evaluated with the standard Bradford method for protein measurement [39].

Biochemistry

Plasma glucose concentrations were measured with a commercial kit (Glucose-PAP Test, Herbos Diagnostics) and the glucose oxidase method. Plasma insulin levels were assessed with a commercial kit for an Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assay (ELISA; Crystal Chem Inc, IL, USA; Cat. No. 90060), which was more sensitive for quantitative analysis (sensitivity <0.8 units/ml) than a Western Blot assay. ELISAs were used to evaluate brain expression levels of the insulin receptor (IR) β-subunit (total), the two forms of the phosphorylated-IRβ subunit, pTyr1162/1163 and pTyr1158 (Calbiochem, Darmstadt, Germany; CBA039, CBA038, and Invitrogen GmbH, Darmstadt, Germany; KHR9121, respectively), the glycogen synthase-kinase (GSK)-3 β-subunit (total), and the phosphorylated form of GSK-3β, pSer9 (Calbiochem, Darmstadt, Germany; CBA068 and CBA069, respectively). All ELISAs were performed as recommended by the manufacturer. In the ELISA tests, brain homogenates were run in parallel with standards for concentration evaluations.

Statistical analysis

The statistical analyses of all data were performed with StatView for Windows (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA; Version 9.3). Data from the MWM training trials were analyzed by fitting general(ized) linear mixed models (Poisson link for the number of errors, normal for escape latency. Instead of adjustment for multiple comparisons, which we considered too conservative, main effects (across all days or both genders/strains) of strain and gender are given with 95 % confidence interval (CI); contrasts arising from interactions with up to 4 cross-products (strain*gender across all days, strain*age across all days) are given with 97.5 % CI; contrasts arising from strain*(sex or age)*day interaction are given with 99 % CI (the significance level p < 0.05, <0.025 or <0.01, respectively). Additionally, the multifactorial analysis of variance (MANOVA), followed by the analysis of Post-Hoc Scheffé test, and the Mann–Whitney U-test, was used. The significance level was set to p <0.05.

Results

Cognitive deficits

After demonstrating that SHR rats performed poorly in MWM training trials regardless of the control strain (WKY or Wistar) (p < 0.05), but that the difference was more pronounced compared to WKY (p < 0.05), WKY rats were used as a control in further experiments, which consistently showed that SHRs performed worse regardless of sex (p < 0.05) or age (p < 0.05), as presented in Additional file 2: Figure S2 & Additional file 3: Figure S3.

As we could show that the most pronounce differences in cognitive deficits in training trials were in female SHR compared to female WKY (p < 0.05), and in some extent due to age (p < 0.05) (Additional file 3: Figure S3), the cognitive performance of 3- and 7-month old female SHR and WKY rats was further recorded in the MWM probe trials. Both SHR age groups demonstrated significant deficits in spatial memory; they spent less time swimming in the quadrant from which the platform has been removed (p < 0.0001; Fig. 1a) and made more errors than controls (7-month-old SHRs, p < 0.05; 3-month-old SHRs, p = 0.06; Fig. 1b). Female SHRs of both ages also showed marked deficits in fear-motivated memory in the PAT. They demonstrated much lower post-shock latency times than WKY controls (p < 0.0001, Fig. 1c), with no age-related effect being detected.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Results of Morris Water Maze probe trial and Passive Avoidance test in spontaneously hypertensive rats. Results are shown for 3- and 7-month-old female (a, b, c) animals. Morris Water Maze (MWM) test–probe trial results: a time spent swimming within the targeted quadrant; b number of entries into the wrong quadrant (mistakes). Passive avoidance test (PAT) results: c latency time. Line in the boxes represent the median, whiskers are the 10 and 90 % percentile. Female spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) = White box; Female Wistar Kyoto control rats (WKY) = Grey box. Significant differences were based on the ANOVA Post-Hoc Scheffé; *p < 0.0001 vs. controls; **p < 0.05; T (tendency) 0.05 < p < 0.1; n = 10

Insulin receptor signaling in the brain

Based on the results of cognitive testing, female SHRs were further used to investigate whether cognitive deficits are accompanied by alterations of insulin signaling in the brain, when compared to WKY controls.

Brain IRβ expression levels

SHRs had, in general, lower total IRβ subunit levels than WKY rats in all brain regions, particularly in the hippocampus and striatum (Fig. 2a) but showed age-dependent increment in IRβ level in the frontal cortex only (p < 0.05, Fig. 2a) in contrast to region-dependent variability in the total IRβ levels with aging in WKY controls (frontal cortex–increased /p < 0.05/, hippocampus–decreased /p < 0.05/, striatum–unchanged, in 7- compared to 3-month-old animals, respectively) (Fig. 2a).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Brain insulin receptor-β-subunit levels and activity in spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar Kyoto control rats. (a) Insulin receptor (IR)β-subunit levels and (b) the ratio of IRβ phosphorylated (pTyr1158 and pTyr1162/1163)/total IRβ are shown for different brain regions of 3- and 7-month-old spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHRs = White) and Wistar Kyoto control rats (WKY = Grey). Columns represent the mean ± SEM Significant differences were based on the ANOVA Post-Hoc Scheffé test; *p < 0.0001; **p < 0.05; n = 10

Brain IR activity

IR activity expressed as the ratio of phosphorylated IR (pTyr1162/1163 and pTyr1158) to total IRβ (higher ratio indicating higher IR activity) was significantly increased in all investigated brain regions in 7- compared to the 3-month-old animals in both SHRs and WKYs but the magnitude of age-dependent increment was larger in SHRs than in WKYs (Fig. 2b). Compared to WKYs, SHRs had significantly higher IR activity at both ages in the hippocampus and striatum and at 7-month of age in the frontal cortex (p < 0.05; Fig. 2b).

Brain GSK3β expression levels and activity

Total GSK3β level in the frontal cortex (p < 0.0005), hippocampus (p < 0.0001) and striatum (p < 0.005) was significantly lower in 3- compared to 7-month-old SHRs (Fig. 3a) while WKYs expressed such age-dependent difference only in the hippocampus (p < 0.05; Fig. 3a). Since GSK3β activity is regulated mainly via phosphorylation of the serine-9 residue, we determined its relative activity expressed as the ratio of the phosphorylated (inactive) to total GSK3β form (low ratio indirectly suggests high GSK3β activity). Age-dependent tendency of decrement in this ratio (increased GSK3β activity) was observed in both SHRs and WKYs with significance reached only in 7-month-old animals but with some regional specificity; SHRs - frontal cortex and striatum (p < 0.05), WKY - frontal cortex (p < 0.005) and hippocampus (p < 0.0001) (Fig. 3b).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Brain glycogen synthase-kinase-3 β-subunit levels and activity in spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar Kyoto control rats. (a) Glycogen synthase-kinase-3 β-subunit (GSK3β) levels and (b) the ratio of GSK3β phosphorylated (pSer9)/GSK3β total are shown for different brain regions of 3- and 7-month-old spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHRs = White) and Wistar Kyoto control rats (WKY = Grey). Columns represent the mean ± SEM. Significant differences were based on the ANOVA Post-Hoc Scheffé test; *p < 0.005; **p < 0.05; 0.05 < T < 0.1; n = 10

Plasma glucose and insulin levels

Both 3- and 7-month-old SHRs showed a significant increase in plasma glucose after 30 and 60 min compared to the baseline levels in the oral glucose tolerance test (OGTT; Fig. 4a and b). A similar time-pattern effect was detected in WKYs with the exception of the 60 min time-point in 7-month-old WKYs when glucose levels were not significantly different from the respective baseline values (Fig. 4a and b). In contrast, the time-course of the plasma insulin levels showed opposite effects in the 3- and 7-month-old rats. In 3-month-old animals, SHRs showed only modest increase in plasma insulin with borderline significant differences at 60 min compared to the baseline levels, while insulin plasma levels in WKYs were significantly increased at both time-points compared to their baseline level (Fig. 4c). In the 7-month-old animals, both the SHRs and WKYs showed significant increases in plasma insulin at the 60 min time-point only, compared to their baseline levels (Fig. 4d). Analysis of the area under the curve (AUC) values of plasma glucose and insulin (Fig. 4e-f) in SHRs and WKYs of both ages pointed to an age-dependent disruption of glucose metabolism for both groups. But to the contrary, plasma insulin AUC was significantly increased with age in the SHR animals, while no significant age-dependent difference was observed in WKY controls.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Glucose challenge results for 3- and 7-month-old rats. Plasma glucose and insulin levels were measured at baseline, 30 min and 60 min after the glucose tolerance test. a Plasma glucose levels in 3-month-old spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR, continuous line + black circle) and Wistar Kyoto controls (WKY, dashed line + white circle); b plasma glucose levels in 7-month-old SHRs and controls; c plasma insulin levels in 3-month-old SHRs and controls; d plasma insulin levels in 7-month-old SHRs and controls; e-f Area under the curve (AUC) of glucose and insulin values from the oral glucose tolerance test (OGTT) of SHR and WKY in 3- and 7-months old animals (points & lines bars, respectively). Bars represent the mean ± SEM. Significant differences were based on the ANOVA Post-Hoc Scheffé; *p < 0.05 vs. baseline control; **p < 0.05 vs. baseline SHR; ***p < 0.05 vs. 60 min control; + p < 0.05 vs. 30 min control; (e & f) + p < 0.05; ++ p < 0.01; n = 10

Discussion

Consistent with previous reports [7, 4042], this study confirmed that the SHR model develops significant memory and learning deficits, which was a prerequisite for the study’s main aim: to explore whether cognitive deficits in SHRs are accompanied by peripheral/central metabolic abnormalities. Considering literature data discussing that selection of corresponding control group (rat strain and usage of inbred, e.g. Wistar-Kyoto/WKY/, or outbred, e.g. Wistar, Sprague–Dawley/SD/animals) as well as age and sex might affect the results of cognitive testing [23, 25, 43], we investigated the cognitive profiles of the optimal control strain and SHR gender and identified those which demonstrated most pronounced difference between the control and SHR group, to be used in exploring the metabolic changes in this model.

Our experiments (Additional file 2: Figure S2 & Additional file 3: Figure S3) provided evidence that the SHR model performed poorly compared to control regardless sex, but females performed worse than males. Some literature data have suggested that hyperactivity behavior might influence the cognitive performance of SHRs and that locomotor activity (distraction) could be a confounding factor in spatial memory tasks [24], for example, in hyperactive female SHRs [32]. Although increased locomotor activity might act as a distraction in achieving a goal, hyperactivity in a cognitively normal rat might also lead to acheiving this goal in a more focused, direct way than in a rat with normal locomotor activity (which was not the case in our experiments). The present data (Additional file 2: Figure S2), however, strongly suggest that the observed deficit manifested as an increased number of errors was not due to hyperactivity: compared to controls, SHR rats made more errors (Additional file 2: Figure S2a, d) and had longer latency times (Additional file 2: Figure S2b, e), but time spent per one error was the same in all strains (Additional file 2: Figure S2c), and with adjustment for the number of errors, latency times did not differ between the SHR and control strains (Additional file 2: Figure S2F). Had hyperactivity (as a distraction factor) been the cause of the increased error rate, one would have expected to see reduced latency times per one error (e.g., faster swimming due to locomotor hyperactivity), or extended latency times per error (e.g., “non-focused” swimming due to hyperactivity), but not the same latency time per error. Since cognitive deficits in our experiments with cognitively more impaired female SHRs were more pronounced in comparison to WKY than to Wistar animals, female SHRs and WKYs were used for further exploration of metabolic abnormalities. WKY and Wistar strains have been frequently used as a control for memory testing in SHRs [22, 23, 25, 43, 44], but usually not in the same experiment with the exemption of the work of Gattu et al. [45, 46] which showed cognitive impairment of 1- and 3-month-old male SHRs compared to both these control strains.

A growing body of evidence points to the association of cognitive deficits and insulin resistance in humans and in animals [4749], an issue that has been poorly explored in the SHR model; a presence of systemic insulin resistance in SHRs has been manifested by a plasma insulin response in 7 week-old animals (sex not specified) [2], and indirectly by a reduced insulin-stimulated glucose transporter bioavailability in adipocytes [50], while an unchanged expression of insulin receptor in the brain of 3-month-old male SHRs was reported by Yang et al. [51]. Our results indicate that the plasma insulin response to an oral glucose challenge is altered in SHRs compared to WKY but that the direction of change depends on a respective time-point of its measurement; lower plasma insulin response in 3-month (AUC = 25/SHR vs. 60/WKY) and greater in 7-month-old female SHRs (AUC = 58/SHR vs. 45/WKY) despite the similarity in the total serum glucose response in SHRs and WKYs. Such a difference in insulin AUC after OGTT was not detected in a recent study investigating various combinations of metabolic syndrome and AD markers in male SHR animals [51], indicating that a possible gender influence in this model might play a role not only in cognition but also in insulin response. Additionally, it could not be excluded that this time-dependent insulin response might correlate with an observed tendency for age-dependent worsening of learning and memory functions in female SHRs, indirectly also reflecting a difference in metabolic response between early (3-month-old) and chronic (7-month-old) hypertension.

Although several studies have suggested that learning and memory deficits observed in SHR animals were independent of hypertension [30, 40, 52], others did find correlations between hypertension and cognitive deficits [5, 6, 41, 53]. Having in mind that aging, female gender, and hypertension are considered risk factors for dementia, particularly of the Alzheimer’s type (see meta-analysis in www.alzrisk.org and [5457], usage of aged SHR animals with reduced cognitive performance might be worth considering as a possible model of a certain type of dementia [5, 58].

Several findings in SHR animals point to their susceptibility to metabolic alterations in the brain. For example, recent findings of Ritz et al. [59] showed that gene expression alterations in the cortex of 2-month-old (pre-hypertension) and 9-month-old SHRs (compared to WKY) pointed to a vulnerability to high-energy requirements in these animal models. Furthermore, studies of cerebral glucose utilization in SHR compared to WKY were found to be lower at aged animals (around 6 months) [60, 61] while not different in younger ones (12 weeks only) [62, 63]. Gene expression profiling in cell cultures originating from the brain stems of SHR and WKY animals revealed significantly enriched genes belonging to molecular pathways of the ATPase activity [64], further supporting the involvement of energy metabolism dysfunction in this model.

Glucose/energy hypometabolism in the brain might be a consequence of an insulin resistant brain state [65, 66], which seems to be involved in cognitive decline, as suggested in particular in AD and its animal models [911, 47, 67]. This hypothesis has been recently discussed for AD patients pointing to the pathophysiological importance of the vicious cycle of glucose/energy demand, vascular cognitive impairment, dementia, and aging, suggesting that an understanding of how vascular and metabolic factors interfere with a progressive loss of functional neuronal networks becomes essential for developing efficient drugs to prevent cognitive decline in the elderly [68]. Keeping in mind that glucose metabolism in the brain is under control of brain IR [12], our findings of dysfunction in brain IR and its downstream signaling cascade might contribute to the pathophysiological mechanisms of the cerebral glucose hypometabolism in SHRs reported in the literature [60]. We generally found lower total levels of the IRβ subunit in SHRs compared to WKYs in all brain regions investigated, regardless of a rat’s age. This finding might indicate that SHRs have a general deficit in brain insulin signaling pathways. Furthermore, we observed an increased IR activity in the 7-month-old SHRs, measured as the ratio of phosphorylated to total IRβ subunits, which might represent a compensatory mechanism covering for reduced IRβ level. Yang and colleagues [51] could not find difference in IR, which, however, was measured in the whole brain (and not in specific brain regions as in our experiment) of young male SHR animals compared to WKY. Our findings demonstrated that GSK3β activity (indirectly measured by p/total GSK3β ratio), which is down-stream to IR signaling and is linked to hyperphosphorylation of tau-protein [69], increased with aging in the cerebral cortex of both the SHR and WKY group, while in the hippocampus it was significantly increased only in 7-month-old female SHRs compared to corresponding WKYs. In recent years, the over-activation of GSK3β in the brain has been reported to be involved in the pathophysiology of AD and of type-2 diabetes mellitus [70].

These results further support the hypothesis that the SHR model develops insulin resistance in the brain, manifested both at the level of IR protein/activity and its downstream signaling pathway, as well at the periphery. Moreover, our results obtained in the same pool of animals demonstrate for the first time the co-existence of hypertension with peripheral and central insulin resistance and cognitive deficits in SHR model.

Conclusions

This study provides the first combined evidence that cognitive deficits in the SHR model are accompanied by insulin-signaling dysfunction in the brain in parallel to the existence of peripheral insulin resistance. Factors like rat strain used for comparison, SHRs age and/or hypertension duration as well as sex should be considered in the interpretation of cognitive and metabolic performance of the SHR model. Further studies are required to address the question of a possible causal relationship between these metabolic and cognitive impairments in SHRs and accordingly explore whether SHRs might be also considered as a model of insulin resistance-induced type of dementia, in addition to being a model of hypertension and ADHD.

Availability of supporting data

The three data sets supporting the results of this article are included within the article (and its additional files). 1) Additional file 1: Detailed cognitive testing methods; 2) Additional file 2: Figure S2: Comparison of the cognitive performance in the Morris Water Maze (MWM) training trials; 3) Additional file 3: Figure S3: The effect of sex and age on the cognitive performance of spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR).

Abbreviations

AD:

Alzheimer’s disease

ADHD:

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

AUC:

Area under the curve

CBF:

Cerebral blood flow

CI:

Confidence interval

CNS:

Central nervous system

ELISA:

Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assay

GSK3:

Glycogen synthase-kinase-3

IR:

Insulin receptor

IRβ:

IR β-subunit

MWM:

Morris Water Maze

MANOVA:

Multifactorial analysis of variance

OGTT:

Oral glucose tolerance test

PAT:

Passive Avoidance Test

Akt/PKB:

Protein kinase B

SHR:

Spontaneously hypertensive rat

SD:

Sprague–Dawley

STZ:

Streptozotocin

WKY:

Wistar-Kyoto rat strain

References

  1. Okamoto K, Aoki K, Nosaka S, Fukushima M. Cardiovascular diseases in the spontaneously hypertensive rat. Jpn Circ J. 1964;28:943–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Mondon CE, Reaven GM. Evidence of abnormalities of insulin metabolism in rats with spontaneous hypertension. Metabolism. 1988;37(4):303–5. doi:10.1016/0026-0495(88)90127-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Sagvolden T, Johansen EB, Woien G, Walaas SI, Storm-Mathisen J, Bergersen LH, et al. The spontaneously hypertensive rat model of ADHD--the importance of selecting the appropriate reference strain. Neuropharmacology. 2009;57(7-8):619–26. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.08.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Sontag TA, Tucha O, Walitza S, Lange KW. Animal models of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a critical review. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2010;2(1):1–20. doi:10.1007/s12402-010-0019-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Meneses A, Perez-Garcia G, Ponce-Lopez T, Tellez R, Gallegos-Cari A, Castillo C. Spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model for ADHD: a short overview. Rev Neurosci. 2011;22(3):365–71. doi:10.1515/RNS.2011.024.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Sabbatini M, Catalani A, Consoli C, Marletta N, Tomassoni D, Avola R. The hippocampus in spontaneously hypertensive rats: an animal model of vascular dementia? Mech Ageing Dev. 2002;123(5):547–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Terry Jr AV, Hernandez CM, Buccafusco JJ, Gattu M. Deficits in spatial learning and nicotinic-acetylcholine receptors in older, spontaneously hypertensive rats. Neuroscience. 2000;101(2):357–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Meneses A, Castillo C, Ibarra M, Hong E. Effects of aging and hypertension on learning, memory, and activity in rats. Physiol Behav. 1996;60(2):341–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Salkovic-Petrisic M, Osmanovic J, Grünblatt E, Riederer P, Hoyer S. Modeling sporadic Alzheimer’s disease: the insulin resistant brain state generates multiple long-term morphobiological abnormalities including hyperphosphorylated tau protein and amyloid-beta. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;18(4):729–50. doi:10.3233/JAD-2009-1184.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Lester-Coll N, Rivera EJ, Soscia SJ, Doiron K, Wands JR, de la Monte SM. Intracerebral streptozotocin model of type 3 diabetes: relevance to sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2006;9(1):13–33.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. de la Monte SM. Brain insulin resistance and deficiency as therapeutic targets in Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2012;9(1):35–66.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Heni M, Hennige AM, Peter A, Siegel-Axel D, Ordelheide AM, Krebs N, et al. Insulin promotes glycogen storage and cell proliferation in primary human astrocytes. PLoS One. 2011;6(6):e21594. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021594. PONE-D-10-03525.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Liu Y, Liu F, Grundke-Iqbal I, Iqbal K, Gong CX. Deficient brain insulin signalling pathway in Alzheimer’s disease and diabetes. J Pathol. 2011;225(1):54–62. doi:10.1002/path.2912.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Deng Y, Li B, Liu Y, Iqbal K, Grundke-Iqbal I, Gong CX. Dysregulation of insulin signaling, glucose transporters, O-GlcNAcylation, and phosphorylation of tau and neurofilaments in the brain: Implication for Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Pathol. 2009;175(5):2089–98. doi:10.2353/ajpath.2009.090157.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Bartl J, Monoranu CM, Wagner AK, Kolter J, Riederer P, Grunblatt E. Alzheimer’s disease and type 2 diabetes: two diseases, one common link? World J Biol Psychiatry. 2013;14(3):233–40. doi:10.3109/15622975.2011.650204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Qu Z, Jiao Z, Sun X, Zhao Y, Ren J, Xu G. Effects of streptozotocin-induced diabetes on tau phosphorylation in the rat brain. Brain Res. 2011;1383:300–6. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.01.084.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Chen Y, Cao CP, Li CR, Wang W, Zhang D, Han LL, et al. Ghrelin modulates insulin sensitivity and tau phosphorylation in high glucose-induced hippocampal neurons. Biol Pharm Bull. 2010;33(7):1165–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Lopez-Gil X, Amat-Roldan I, Tudela R, Castane A, Prats-Galino A, Planas AM, et al. DWI and complex brain network analysis predicts vascular cognitive impairment in spontaneous hypertensive rats undergoing executive function tests. Front Aging Neurosci. 2014;6:167. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2014.00167.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Shaul ME, Hallacoglu B, Sassaroli A, Shukitt-Hale B, Fantini S, Rosenberg IH, et al. Cerebral blood volume and vasodilation are independently diminished by aging and hypertension: a near infrared spectroscopy study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;42 Suppl 3:S189–98. doi:10.3233/JAD-132504.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Clements KM, Saunders AJ, Robertson BA, Wainwright PE. Spontaneously hypertensive, Wistar Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley rats differ in their use of place and response strategies in the water radial arm maze. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;87(2):285–94. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2006.09.003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Ferguson SA, Cada AM. Spatial learning/memory and social and nonsocial behaviors in the spontaneously hypertensive, Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley rat strains. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;77(3):583–94. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2003.12.014.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Langen B, Dost R. Comparison of SHR, WKY and Wistar rats in different behavioural animal models: effect of dopamine D1 and alpha2 agonists. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2011;3(1):1–12. doi:10.1007/s12402-010-0034-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Robertson BA, Clements KM, Wainwright PE. The working memory capabilities of the spontaneously hypertensive rat. Physiol Behav. 2008;94(3):481–6. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.02.016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Sontag TA, Fuermaier AB, Hauser J, Kaunzinger I, Tucha O, Lange KW. Spatial memory in spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR). PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e74660. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0074660.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Wyss JM, Chambless BD, Kadish I, van Groen T. Age-related decline in water maze learning and memory in rats: strain differences. Neurobiol Aging. 2000;21(5):671–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. van den Bergh FS, Bloemarts E, Chan JS, Groenink L, Olivier B, Oosting RS. Spontaneously hypertensive rats do not predict symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2006;83(3):380–90. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2006.02.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Chelaru MI, Yang PB, Dafny N. Sex differences in the behavioral response to methylphenidate in three adolescent rat strains (WKY, SHR, SD). Behav Brain Res. 2012;226(1):8–17. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.08.027.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hendley ED, Wessel DJ, Atwater DG, Gellis J, Whitehorn D, Low WC. Age, sex and strain differences in activity and habituation in SHR and WKY rats. Physiol Behav. 1985;34(3):379–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Pires VA, Pamplona FA, Pandolfo P, Fernandes D, Prediger RD, Takahashi RN. Adenosine receptor antagonists improve short-term object-recognition ability of spontaneously hypertensive rats: a rodent model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav Pharmacol. 2009;20(2):134–45. doi:10.1097/FBP.0b013e32832a80bf.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Prediger RD, Pamplona FA, Fernandes D, Takahashi RN. Caffeine improves spatial learning deficits in an animal model of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) -- the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR). Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;8(4):583–94. doi:10.1017/S1461145705005341.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Robinson AM, Hopkins ME, Bucci DJ. Effects of physical exercise on ADHD-like behavior in male and female adolescent spontaneously hypertensive rats. Dev Psychobiol. 2011;53(4):383–90. doi:10.1002/dev.20530.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Bucci DJ, Hopkins ME, Keene CS, Sharma M, Orr LE. Sex differences in learning and inhibition in spontaneously hypertensive rats. Behav Brain Res. 2008;187(1):27–32. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.08.022.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Andersen SL. Changes in the second messenger cyclic AMP during development may underlie motoric symptoms in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Behav Brain Res. 2002;130(1-2):197–201.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Hinojos CA, Boerwinkle E, Fornage M, Doris PA. Combined genealogical, mapping, and expression approaches to identify spontaneously hypertensive rat hypertension candidate genes. Hypertension. 2005;45(4):698–704. doi:10.1161/01.HYP.0000156498.78896.37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Luo Y, Owens D, Mulder G, McVey A, Fisher T. Blood Pressure Characterization of Hypertensive and Control Rats for Cardiovascular Studies. AHA, Atlanta: Charles River; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Salkovic-Petrisic M, Osmanovic-Barilar J, Knezovic A, Hoyer S, Mosetter K, Reutter W. Long-term oral galactose treatment prevents cognitive deficits in male Wistar rats treated intracerebroventricularly with streptozotocin. Neuropharmacology. 2014;77:68–80. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.09.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Salkovic-Petrisic M, Tribl F, Schmidt M, Hoyer S, Riederer P. Alzheimer-like changes in protein kinase B and glycogen synthase kinase-3 in rat frontal cortex and hippocampus after damage to the insulin signalling pathway. J Neurochem. 2006;96(4):1005–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Compact sixth edition ed. London, UK: Elsevier Inc.; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Bradford MM. Rapid and sensitive method for quantitation of microgram quantities of proteins utilizing principle of protein dye binding. Anal Biochem. 1976;72:248–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Kantak KM, Singh T, Kerstetter KA, Dembro KA, Mutebi MM, Harvey RC, et al. Advancing the spontaneous hypertensive rat model of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Neurosci. 2008;122(2):340–57. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.122.2.340.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Hernandez CM, Hoifodt H, Terry Jr AV. Spontaneously hypertensive rats: further evaluation of age-related memory performance and cholinergic marker expression. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2003;28(3):197–209.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Terry Jr AV, Buccafusco JJ. The cholinergic hypothesis of age and Alzheimer’s disease-related cognitive deficits: recent challenges and their implications for novel drug development. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;306(3):821–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Alsop B. Reprint of “Problems with spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) as a model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD)”. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;166(2):XV–XXI. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.12.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Bucci DJ, Hopkins ME, Nunez AA, Breedlove SM, Sisk CL, Nigg JT. Effects of sex hormones on associative learning in spontaneously hypertensive rats. Physiol Behav. 2008;93(3):651–7. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Gattu M, Pauly JR, Boss KL, Summers JB, Buccafusco JJ. Cognitive impairment in spontaneously hypertensive rats: role of central nicotinic receptors. I Brain Res. 1997;771(1):89–103.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Gattu M, Terry Jr AV, Pauly JR, Buccafusco JJ. Cognitive impairment in spontaneously hypertensive rats: role of central nicotinic receptors. Part II. Brain Res. 1997;771(1):104–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. De Felice FG, Lourenco MV, Ferreira ST. How does brain insulin resistance develop in Alzheimer’s disease? Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10(1S):S26–32. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2013.12.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Dineley KT, Jahrling JB, Denner L. Insulin resistance in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Dis. 2014;72(Pt A):92–103. doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2014.09.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Crichton GE, Elias MF, Buckley JD, Murphy KJ, Bryan J, Frisardi V. Metabolic syndrome, cognitive performance, and dementia. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;30 Suppl 2:S77–87. doi:10.3233/JAD-2011-111022.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Collison M, James DJ, Graham D, Holman GD, Connell JM, Dominiczak AF, et al. Reduced insulin-stimulated GLUT4 bioavailability in stroke-prone spontaneously hypertensive rats. Diabetologia. 2005;48(3):539–46. doi:10.1007/s00125-005-1674-x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Yang HT, Sheen YJ, Kao CD, Chang CA, Hu YC, Lin JL. Association between the characteristics of metabolic syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease. Metab Brain Dis. 2013;28(4):597–604. doi:10.1007/s11011-013-9406-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Loskutova LV, Dubrovina NI, Markel AL. Comparative analysis of the persistence of a conditioned passive avoidance reflex in rats with different forms of inherited hypertension. Neurosci Behav Physiol. 2007;37(6):577–82. doi:10.1007/s11055-007-0055-y.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Chan SH, Wu KL, Kung PS, Chan JY. Oral intake of rosiglitazone promotes a central antihypertensive effect via upregulation of peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor-gamma and alleviation of oxidative stress in rostral ventrolateral medulla of spontaneously hypertensive rats. Hypertension. 2010;55(6):1444–53. doi:10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.149146.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Power MC, Weuve J, Gagne JJ, McQueen MB, Viswanathan A, Blacker D. The association between blood pressure and incident Alzheimer disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Epidemiology. 2011;22(5):646–59. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e31822708b5.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. World Health Organization. Dementia. World Health Organization. 2012. http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs362/en/index.html. Accessed 17.09.2012 2012.

  56. Azad NA, Al Bugami M, Loy-English I. Gender differences in dementia risk factors. Gend Med. 2007;4(2):120–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Mukhtar O, Jackson SH. The hypertension in the very elderly trial - latest data. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2013;75(4):951–4. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2125.2012.04427.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Tayebati SK, Tomassoni D, Amenta F. Spontaneously hypertensive rat as a model of vascular brain disorder: microanatomy, neurochemistry and behavior. J Neurol Sci. 2012;322(1-2):241–9. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2012.05.047.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Ritz MF, Grond-Ginsbach C, Engelter S, Lyrer P. Gene expression suggests spontaneously hypertensive rats may have altered metabolism and reduced hypoxic tolerance. Curr Neurovasc Res. 2012;9(1):10–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Wei L, Lin SZ, Tajima A, Nakata H, Acuff V, Patlak C, et al. Cerebral glucose utilization and blood flow in adult spontaneously hypertensive rats. Hypertension. 1992;20(4):501–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Johansson BB. Pentoxifylline: cerebral blood flow and glucose utilization in conscious spontaneously hypertensive rats. Stroke. 1986;17(4):744–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Orzi F, Morisco C, Colangelo V, Di Grezia R, Lembo G. Lack of effect of insulin on glucose utilization of the hypothalamus in normotensive and hypertensive rats. Neurosci Lett. 2000;278(1-2):29–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Kadekaro M, Savaki HE, Kutyna FA, Davidsen L, Sokoloff L. Metabolic mapping in the sympathetic ganglia and brain of the spontaneously hypertensive rat. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1983;3(4):460–7. doi:10.1038/jcbfm.1983.72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Ferrari MF, Reis EM, Matsumoto JP, Fior-Chadi DR. Gene expression profiling of cultured cells from brainstem of newborn spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar Kyoto rats. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2009;29(3):287–308. doi:10.1007/s10571-008-9321-y.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Sievenpiper JL, Jenkins AL, Whitham DL, Vuksan V. Insulin resistance: concepts, controversies, and the role of nutrition. Can J Diet Pract Res. 2002;63(1):20–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Hoyer S. Glucose metabolism and insulin receptor signal transduction in Alzheimer disease. Eur J Pharmacol. 2004;490(1-3):115–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Craft S, Cholerton B, Baker LD. Insulin and Alzheimer’s disease: untangling the web. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;33 Suppl 1:S263–75. doi:10.3233/JAD-2012-129042.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Popa-Wagner A, Buga AM, Popescu B, Muresanu D. Vascular cognitive impairment, dementia, aging and energy demand. A vicious cycle. J Neural Transm. 2013;In Print. doi:10.1007/s00702-013-1129-3.

  69. Hernandez F, Lucas JJ, Avila J. GSK3 and tau: two convergence points in Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;33 Suppl 1:S141–4. doi:10.3233/JAD-2012-129025.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Gao C, Holscher C, Liu Y, Li L. GSK3: a key target for the development of novel treatments for type 2 diabetes mellitus and Alzheimer disease. Rev Neurosci. 2011;23(1):1–11. doi:10.1515/rns.2011.061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

The authors would like to thank the “Bundesprogramm Chancengleichheit” and the national project, financed by the Croatian Ministry of Science and Education (MZOS), for their support. We also extend special thanks to Miss Miryame Hofmann and Mrs. Bozica Hrzan for technical assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Edna Grünblatt.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JB, DII carried out the ELISA assays and performed the statistical analysis. AK carried out the behavior tests. VT performed the statistical analysis. PR & SW participated in the design of the study, in the data interpretation and helped to draft the manuscript. EG and MSP conceived the study, participated in its design and coordination and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

Susanne Walitza and Melita Salkovic-Petrisic have equal senior authorship.

Additional files

Additional file 1:

Detailed cognitive testing methods.

Additional file 2:

Comparison of the cognitive performance in the Morris Water Maze (MWM) training trials.

Additional file 3:

The effect of sex and age on the cognitive performance of spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR).

Rights and permissions

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Grünblatt, E., Bartl, J., Iuhos, DI. et al. Characterization of cognitive deficits in spontaneously hypertensive rats, accompanied by brain insulin receptor dysfunction. J Mol Psychiatr 3, 6 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40303-015-0012-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40303-015-0012-6

Keywords